认可容易被证伪的、与优先考虑象征性力量相关的说法。
Endorsing easily disproven claims linked to prioritizing symbolic strength

原始链接: https://theconversation.com/winning-with-misinformation-new-research-identifies-link-between-endorsing-easily-disproven-claims-and-prioritizing-symbolic-strength-265652

## 为什么人们坚持已被证伪的说法 最新研究揭示了一个令人惊讶的原因,解释了为什么有些人会积极支持容易被证伪的错误信息:他们认为这是一种力量和独立的表现。一项对八个国家超过5500人的研究发现,对COVID-19错误信息的相信,主要不是由事实理解或政治倾向驱动,而是源于抵制 perceived 外部影响的愿望。 那些在“象征性力量”心态上得分较高的人——同意诸如“遵循指导意味着退缩”之类的陈述——将表现出反抗精神置于事实准确性之上。对他们来说,支持虚假信息不是因为他们相信这些信息,而是为了发出抵抗信号,并在与对立力量的心理战中“获胜”。这种模式超出了COVID-19的范畴,也出现在对加密货币甚至阴谋论的信念中,并与威权态度相关。 该研究表明,直接驳斥错误信息对这部分人可能无效,因为他们会将纠正他们的尝试视为软弱。相反,顽固地坚持已被证伪的说法*本身*就是目的——一种强烈的独立声明。这突显了,对于某些人来说,意义并非来自真相,而是来自象征性定位以及对信念的 perceived 心理战。

## 黑客新闻讨论摘要:象征性力量与虚假信息 一篇近期文章探讨了一种观点,即人们认同虚假信息,不一定是因为追求*真相*,而是出于表达忠诚和在 perceived 心理战中“获胜”的愿望。核心论点是,公开支持易于被证伪的说法,可以作为一种展示力量的方式,并阻止“敌人”取得进展,即使这些说法本身无关紧要。 讨论主要集中在这一观点是否具有新颖性,许多人指出,关于部落主义和虚假新闻传播的类似观察已经记录多年。一些人认为这篇文章过于简化了问题,动机更加复杂——包括想要相信某事是真的,或者引发情绪反应。 一个关键的争论点是,研究是否偏离了中立,通过关注像唐纳德·特朗普这样的例子,进入党派政治,一些人指责作者在“用科学包装”政治观点。另一些人则捍卫将心理学发现应用于现实世界的政治现象的合理性,特别是关于相信虚假信息的动机。最终,这场对话凸显了将心理机制与政治和社会背景分离的难度。
相关文章

原文

Why do some people endorse claims that can easily be disproved? It’s one thing to believe false information, but another to actively stick with something that’s obviously wrong.

Our new research, published in the Journal of Social Psychology, suggests that some people consider it a “win” to lean in to known falsehoods.

We are social psychologists who study political psychology and how people reason about reality. During the pandemic, we surveyed 5,535 people across eight countries to investigate why people believed COVID-19 misinformation, like false claims that 5G networks cause the virus.

The strongest predictor of whether someone believed in COVID-19-related misinformation and risks related to the vaccine was whether they viewed COVID-19 prevention efforts in terms of symbolic strength and weakness. In other words, this group focused on whether an action would make them appear to fend off or “give in” to untoward influence.

This factor outweighed how people felt about COVID-19 in general, their thinking style and even their political beliefs.

Our survey measured it on a scale of how much people agreed with sentences including “Following coronavirus prevention guidelines means you have backed down” and “Continuous coronavirus coverage in the media is a sign we are losing.” Our interpretation is that people who responded positively to these statements would feel they “win” by endorsing misinformation – doing so can show “the enemy” that it will not gain any ground over people’s views.

When meaning is symbolic, not factual

Rather than consider issues in light of actual facts, we suggest people with this mindset prioritize being independent from outside influence. It means you can justify espousing pretty much anything – the easier a statement is to disprove, the more of a power move it is to say it, as it symbolizes how far you’re willing to go.

When people think symbolically this way, the literal issue – here, fighting COVID-19 – is secondary to a psychological war over people’s minds. In the minds of those who think they’re engaged in them, psychological wars are waged over opinions and attitudes, and are won via control of belief and messaging. The U.S. government at various times has used the concept of psychological war to try to limit the influence of foreign powers, pushing people to think that literal battles are less important than psychological independence.

By that same token, vaccination, masking or other COVID-19 prevention efforts could be seen as a symbolic risk that could “weaken” one psychologically even if they provide literal physical benefits. If this seems like an extreme stance, it is – the majority of participants in our studies did not hold this mindset. But those who did were especially likely to also believe in misinformation.

In an additional study we ran that focused on attitudes around cryptocurrency, we measured whether people saw crypto investment in terms of signaling independence from traditional finance. These participants, who, like those in our COVID-19 study, prioritized a symbolic show of strength, were more likely to believe in other kinds of misinformation and conspiracies, too, such as that the government is concealing evidence of alien contact.

In all of our studies, this mindset was also strongly associated with authoritarian attitudes, including beliefs that some groups should dominate others and support for autocratic government. These links help explain why strongman leaders often use misinformation symbolically to impress and control a population.

Attempts to debunk misinformation look weak to someone who values a symbolic show of strength, while standing by a disprovable statement seems powerful. Anna Moneymaker/Getty Images

Why people endorse misinformation

Our findings highlight the limits of countering misinformation directly, because for some people, literal truth is not the point.

For example, President Donald Trump incorrectly claimed in August 2025 that crime in Washington D.C. was at an all-time high, generating countless fact-checks of his premise and think pieces about his dissociation from reality.

But we believe that to someone with a symbolic mindset, debunkers merely demonstrate that they’re the ones reacting, and are therefore weak. The correct information is easily available, but is irrelevant to someone who prioritizes a symbolic show of strength. What matters is signaling one isn’t listening and won’t be swayed.

In fact, for symbolic thinkers, nearly any statement should be justifiable. The more outlandish or easily disproved something is, the more powerful one might seem when standing by it. Being an edgelord – a contrarian online provocateur – or outright lying can, in their own odd way, appear “authentic.”

Some people may also view their favorite dissembler’s claims as provocative trolling, but, given the link between this mindset and authoritarianism, they want those far-fetched claims acted on anyway. The deployment of National Guard troops to Washington, for example, can be the desired end goal, even if the offered justification is a transparent farce.

Is this really 5-D chess?

It is possible that symbolic, but not exactly true, beliefs have some downstream benefit, such as serving as negotiation tactics, loyalty tests, or a fake-it-till-you-make-it long game that somehow, eventually, becomes a reality. Political theorist Murray Edelman, known for his work on political symbolism, noted that politicians often prefer scoring symbolic points over delivering results – it’s easier. Leaders can offer symbolism when they have little tangible to provide.

联系我们 contact @ memedata.com